Cookies

We use cookies to ensure that we give you the best experience on our website. You can change your cookie settings at any time. Otherwise, we'll assume you're OK to continue.

Durham University

Durham University News

News

Did our brains evolve to foolishly follow celebrities?

(26 June 2013)

Dr Jamie Tehrani

Dr Jamie TehraniAnthropology, argues that our obsession with celebrity culture is a result of our maladapted brains.

First published on BBC News Magazine

One of my all-time favourite quotes comes from Mark Twain, who once wrote to his friend “I am sorry for the length of this letter, but I did not have the time to write a short one”. It’s an apology I have often repeated in bloated ramblings to my friends and colleagues, and it’s a wonderfully wry, pithy insight. Typical Twain, you might say. Except that it’s not. Because, as the person who recently pulled me up for using it told me, the true author of the quote is in fact a less well-known French thinker, Blaise Pascal, who coined it in a letter to a colleague in 1657. I looked it up and they were absolutely right. And it turns out not to be the only quote I’ve been abusing. I’m sure most of you are familiar with Einstein’s brilliant refrain “the definition of insanity is doing the same thing over and over again and expecting a different result”. It’s probably the most famous thing he said apart from “E=mc2”. Only, there’s no record of him every having uttered these words. The first time they appeared in print was in 1981, in a Narcotics Anonymous pamphlet, some 25 years after the great man died.

There are many, many similar examples. Churchill, Benjamin Franklin and Martin Luther King have probably said less than half the things you’ve heard them quoted on. Because quotes are just so much more quotable when they come from individuals who are famed for their wit and wisdom. Which is why I know that if you remember anything that I say today, and find an opportune moment to bring it into conversation, you’ll tell your friends you heard it on a Radio 4 show with Brian Cox, or someone. It’s OK. Because misattributing quotes exemplifies the phenomenon I want to talk about today – our tendency to give too much credit to celebrities.

Fame is a powerful cultural magnet. As a hyper-social species, we acquire the bulk of our knowledge, ideas and skills by copying from others, rather than through individual trial-and-error. However, we pay far more attention to the habits and behaviours demonstrated by famous people than those demonstrated by ordinary members of our community. It follows that things are much more likely to catch on if they are associated with someone who is well known for one reason or another – even if the association is erroneous, as in the case of those Twain and Einstein misquotations. You have to ask yourself, how many of us would have come across that definition of insanity if it were only known from the Narcotics Anonymous pamphlet? Probably very few. Which raises the question of whether what is said is as important as who said it.

Another example of the way in which celebrities act as cultural magnets is that we frequently copy traits that have little, if anything, to do with what made them successful in the first place – like the clothes they wear, their hairstyles, or how they talk. That’s basically the reason that companies sponsor stars to use their products. Celebrities are always on the TV and in the media, so of course getting them to wear your brand of jeans or wristwatch is a great way to give them exposure. But it’s not just about getting your products in the public eye. You wouldn’t know from images on TV or in a newspaper or computer what kind of underpants David Beckham wears, what coffee George Clooney drinks, or what perfume Beyonce smells of. Companies get celebrities to advertise these kinds of products because they know that our perceptions of value are actively influenced by fame. Celebrity endorsements not only make products more visible, they make them more desirable.

So why is this? Celebrity culture is often portrayed as something relatively new, a product of a media-saturated but socially atomised society. Although I agree that the form of celebrity culture has no doubt been shaped by the modern world, I want to argue that it is rooted in much more basic human instincts, which have played a key role in our acquisition of culture and have been crucial to the evolutionary success of our species.

Specifically, I am going to focus on the anthropology of prestige. Prestige is a form of social status that is based on the respect or admiration of members of one’s community. It is particularly interesting for anthropologists because it seems to be a unique characteristic of our species, and something that is universal to all human cultures. In other primates, social hierarchies are typically based on dominance, which is different from prestige because it implies fear, and the threat of violence. Individuals defer to more dominant animals because if they fail to let them have what they want then it would be perceived as a challenge to their status, which they will defend by force. Many types of hierarchy in human society are similarly characterised by dominance. However, unlike other primates, we also differentiate social status in terms of prestige. In contrast to dominance, prestige is given voluntarily. It is freely conferred to individuals in recognition of their achievements in a particular field, and is not backed up by force.

How did such systems arise? The most convincing theory suggests that prestige evolved as part of a package of psychological adaptations for cultural learning. It allowed our ancestors to recognise and reward individuals with superior skills and knowledge, and learn from them. Socially recognised prestige – or “proto-fame”, if you like – was a cue that guided people towards the best role models in their communities. This allowed new discoveries and techniques – for instance, how to exploit the medicinal properties of plants or optimise the design of hunting weapons – to spread across the whole population, and enabled each successive generation to build on and improve the knowledge of their predecessors. This capacity appears to be singularly lacking in our nearest primate relatives, despite their impressive abilities to make and use tools. For instance, there is a population of wild chimpanzees who have learnt how to crack open nuts by hammering them with a stone against a flat anvil. Some of the researchers who study them observed one of the animals discover a superior technique, which involved stabilising the anvil by placing a small rock underneath it. However, like other similar innovations in chimp technology, it never caught on. Why? Because in a dominance-fixated society, skill and inventiveness receive little recognition. Brawn beats brains.

Of course, the benefits of prestige-biased learning do not all flow one way. In return for sharing their knowledge, prestigious individuals enjoy many social advantages over their peers. Often, they are exempted from the social norms that everybody else is expected to abide by. For instance, in hunter-gatherer societies, highly esteemed hunters often get more than their fair share of meat, despite an “official” ideology that game is distributed equally among all members of the community. It’s a kind of forager equivalent to someone famous turning up to a top end restaurant without a booking, and always getting table. They’re not really supposed to do it, but everybody knows they can.

Although the bias for preferentially imitating prestigious individuals has generally helped promote the spread of adaptive behaviours, anthropologists have suggested that it can make us susceptible to copying traits that are of no use in themselves, or which may even be harmful. The reason for this is that prestige-biased learning is a very general strategy that is targeted at successful role models, rather than specific traits. This is precisely what makes it such a powerful and flexible tool – because the traits that make someone successful will vary significantly in different environments, so it makes sense to copy whoever happens to be doing best at a particular time and place. However, because this strategy is somewhat indiscriminate, it can lead to people adopting all kinds of behaviours exhibited by a role model, including ones that have nothing to do with their success. For example, men might observe a successful hunter perform some kind of incantation at the same time as he re-touches his arrowheads, and adopt both the ritual as well as his knapping techniques as a single package when they prepare their own tools. This tendency, I believe, explains our interest in what sports stars and singers wear, what car they drive, and where they go shopping.

In the past any useless traits we acquired as a result of prestige-biased learning were offset by the benefits of picking up useful skills. So, in the long-run, it was an effective, adaptive strategy. However, I am far from convinced that our attraction to prestige continues to promote superior cultural knowledge and skill. The modern world is very different from the one in which our brains evolved, and I believe that the originally adaptive bias for imitating successful people has today morphed into an unhealthy obsession with celebrities, who we give far more attention to than they deserve. Let me illustrate the point by way of an analogy to diet. We have an evolved preference for sweet-tasting and fatty foods because they motivated our ancestors to seek out ripe fruits and meat, which are rich in essential nutrients. But in today’s world of mass-produced confectionary and intensive agriculture, these previously adaptive tastes have led to a massive obesity epidemic. Similarly, we can think of the mass-media as junk food for the mind. Quick. Convenient. But not exactly nutritious. We gorge ourselves on images of wealth and success because they appeal to our appetite for prestige. But are celebrities actually good role-models?

In posing that question, I am not referring to well-publicised instances of bad behaviour by over-sexed footballers or drunken soap stars. I am talking on a more general level about the very purpose of celebrity itself: In ancestral societies, the blueprint for a good role-model was relatively well-defined: a good hunter, or gatherer, or parent, maybe a ritual expert. But in our society, with its complex class system, division of labour and melting pot of cultures, the criteria for success are far more varied and opaque. Someone who may be an excellent role model in my field, an academic anthropologist, would probably be of little use to most of the people in this audience. Similarly, many celebrities have accomplished their success in fields like sport and music, which most of us have little hope of emulating. But we still imitate what we can because our brains are programmed to associate prestige with adaptive behaviour. And because fame is the primary cue of prestige, the more attention celebrities get, the more they attract. It’s not surprising then, that fame has become an end in itself. Because in the modern world, it does not really matter what you are famous for.

Indeed, while celebrities today get more attention and prestige than at any other point in human history, we are frequently being told not to hold them up as role models. But – seen from the evolutionary, anthropological perspective that I have sketched out today – you may ask, what are celebrities for if they are not to be role models? Why give them the benefits of our prestige, if it is not reciprocated with anything that might be of use to us? In pondering those questions, we would do well to reflect on the words of Samuel Johnson “To get a name is one of the few things that cannot be bought. It is the free gift of mankind, which must be deserved before it will be granted”. At least, I think that was Samuel Johnson….

 

Share this story